Skip to main content

The impact of maturation level, not chronological age, on attentional control: implications for sports injury prevention in female adolescents

Abstract

Background

Non-contact injuries are highly prevalent among young athletes and occur particularly in situations that require fast decision making and divided attention. Administering relevant neurocognitive tests could help identify deficiencies in these cognitive abilities and thus potentially mitigate injury risk. However, processes such as selective attention and response inhibition might depend to some extent on the athlete’s maturation stage. We aimed to examine the effect of maturation on selective visual attention and response inhibition among adolescent volleyball players.

Methods

In this cross-sectional study, 52 female adolescents (age 12.3 ± 2.1 years) performed an Eriksen Flankers task. Participants were divided into subgroups based on their estimated adult stature, using the Khamis & Roche method: Pre-pubertal (PRE; n = 13, age: 9.9 ± 1.3), early-puberty (EPUB; n = 7, age: 10.5 ± 0.6), mid-puberty (MPUB; n = 8, age: 12.6 ± 0.8) and late puberty (LPUB; n = 24, age: 14.1 ± 0.9). Analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) were performed on congruent and incongruent reaction times (RT), with corresponding success rates (% correct responses) as covariate. Flanker interference effect was tested using ANOVA. Correlations were further examined between the dependent variables and participants’ chronological age.

Results

There was a significant group effect, with PRE demonstrating longer RT compared with LPUB (P < 0.001) for both congruent and incongruent RT. Moderate negative correlations were observed between age and RT (R= -0.695, R= -0.614 for congruent and incongruent RT, respectively) and low positive correlations between age and incongruent success rate (R= 0.318). Low to moderate correlations were also observed within the LPUB group for RT (R= -0.431–-0.532) and success rate (negative R= -574 for congruent and positive R= 0.417 for incongruent). There were no group differences nor age associations with interference effect.

Conclusions

Our findings indicate that information processing and selective visual attention are superior at late maturation compared with early maturation among female adolescents. The same cannot be said for response inhibition, which did not differ between maturation groups. Similar tendencies were observed with regards to chronological age, but not entirely explained by it. Maturation level, rather than chronological age, should guide practitioners during sport participation and injury prevention programs for young athletes, whose neurocognitive abilities are not yet fully developed, placing them at risk for non-contact injuries.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Major sporting injuries, e.g., anterior cruciate ligament (ACL) tears, are a main concern among athletes and practitioners, with non-contact mechanisms accounting for more than 50% of injuries during team ball sports [1] and a significantly higher risk for injury among females in general [2, 3] and female adolescents in particular [4]. In addition to risk-associated tasks such as landing and cutting manoeuvres [5], a staple of non-contact injuries is the presence of a distractor (e.g., another player in close proximity) [6,7,8,9,10] as well as a need for attentional control – the capacity to choose what to attend to and what to ignore – and fast decision making, both have been shown to influence lower limb biomechanics [11]. Excessively high cognitive demands, which are frequent in the dynamic environment of team ball sports, are thus intensified due to an abundance of visual and somatosensory cues in a rapidly changing environment. Consequently, during game situations, an athlete’s attentional pool may become overloaded, potentially exceeding their capacity and leading to injuries [12]. An athlete must carefully divide their attention in order to cope with game-related demands (e.g., other players, field of play), while maintaining adequate joint stability during complex movements [13]. Selective visual attention, the ability to filter out irrelevant visual information [14], is prudent for athletes who are faced with conflicting information and are required to rapidly react under pressure. Practitioners may need to get a clearer picture of an athlete’s capability to focus their attention on task-relevant information (while gating out the irrelevant) and consequently select appropriate responses. Testing attentional resources along with additional neurocognitive constructs could subsequently help identify potential deficits that may contribute to the susceptibility of an athlete to getting injured.

The rate of ACL injuries is particularly high among adolescents compared to adults [1]. When considering neurocognitive function, previous evidence clearly shows age-dependent functioning in various neuropsychological domains such as attention, executive function, memory and learning, with significant developments occurring at preadolescent [15] and adolescent [16] stages. It has additionally been suggested that maturation stage should be considered in this context, rather than chronological age of child athletes [17, 18]. Given that adolescents mature at different rates [19], it is crucial to investigate the impact of maturation/growth level on neurocognitive abilities, as it may provide valuable insights into the vulnerability of individuals at different developmental stages to non-contact injuries. This consideration is, however, seldom made, leaving practitioners with insufficient information to incorporate neurocognitive training into injury prevention programs for adolescent athletes.

The Eriksen Flanker task [20] is a reliable (test-retest ICC = 0.82–0.86 [21]) and easily administered choice reaction time test to assess an individual’s selective attention and their capability to suppress inappropriate responses to a given stimulus (i.e., response inhibition). The test involves repeated presentations of a central target letter surrounded by flanker letters that can be either congruent or incongruent with the central one. The participant is then required to react to the central target by pressing an appropriate predefined button, while ignoring the surrounding stimuli. Previous work has shown that response inhibition, as measured by Flanker test interference effect, is highly dependent on age, with full maturity suggested to not be reached until the age of 11 [22]. This finding, to some extent, was consistent with a prior study, which reported age-dependent improvements in flanker interference effect on error rates [23]. However, none of the previous studies considered pubertal maturation stage when grouping children in their respective cohorts, which given the potential variability in children’s neurocognitive development, warrants additional investigation. Moreover, relying solely on chronological age can potentially obscure important differences in cognitive abilities. This may negatively impact slowly developing adolescents who are expected to perform at the same as their age-matched peers. In the current study, we administered the Flanker task among a cohort of female volleyball players of different maturation levels, estimated based on predicted adult stature using the method developed by Khamis & Roche (1994) [24]. This approach utilizes a regression equation incorporating current stature, current body mass, and the average stature of both biological parents to predict the eventual adult stature of the child. We aimed to assess the effect of maturation status on reaction times (RT), success rate and flanker interference effect, with implications for future non-contact injuries among physically active female adolescents. We hypothesised that more mature adolescent would demonstrate better RT and success rates. As a secondary aim, we assessed these same outcomes in relation to chronological age. We hypothesised similar tendencies with respect to age, with better Flankers test performance among older participants.

Methods

Study design

This was a cross-sectional study, performed in a controlled testing environment setting within the sports hall of the BALUO Application Centre, Olomouc, Czech Republic. The study received approval from the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Physical Culture, Palacký University Olomouc (reference number: 15/2023) and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent documents were obtained from all guardians of the participants, after providing them with detailed information about the potential risks associated with the testing procedures.

Participants

Recruitment of test participants took place during February 2023. The necessary sample size was predetermined using G*Power version 3.1.9.7 [25]. The analysis was based on effect size from previous study examining the differences of reaction time between early and late adolescent groups [26], a statistical significance level α = 0.05 and power 1-β = 0.95. The result indicated a required sample of 40 participants. To accommodate a potential 50% disagreement in study participation, as well as potential withdrawals and missing data, we approached 80 participants. All participants were female volleyball players 7 to 15 years old from the same club, engaged in training three times a week. Participation required meeting the following inclusion criteria: (a) active engagement in competitive volleyball; (b) absence of significant orthopaedic injuries (e.g., sprains, fractures, and tears) for a minimum of three months before the study commencement; (c) absence of any pain that might restrict participation. As our target population were adolescents with typical development, individuals with a diagnosed neurodevelopmental disorder were excluded. Participants were recruited using the following process: a researcher approached the main coach who then approached the other divisional coaches to explain the study. They were then responsible for primary screening of athletes. Eligible athletes were given an information sheet describing the aim of the research and the testing requirements. Out of a total of 80 participants contacted, 56 agreed to participate in the research. Three participants were then excluded due to missing data on biological parents’ height, which was crucial for group allocation (see below), and an additional participant was later excluded for being a statistical outlier, which resulted in a final sample size of 52 participants.

Testing procedures

Anthropometrics and maturity measures

Data collection occurred during two consecutive testing days in the month of March 2023. It included an extensive test battery comprised comprising performance-related functional tests (e.g., drop-jumps, dynamic balance) and testing for injury-related fisk factors. Maturity calculations according to Khamis & Roche (1994) [24] were performed with the aid of Microsoft Excel spreadsheets created by Towlson (2020) [18] and were used to define the study groups. The assessment categorised participants into different maturation stages based on the percentage of predicted adult stature attained. Specifically, ranges of < 85%, ≥ 85% to < 90%, ≥ 90% to < 95%, and ≥ 95% were indicative of pre-pubertal (PRE; n = 13), early pubertal (EPUB; n = 7), mid-pubertal (MPUB; n = 8) and late-pubertal (LPUB; n = 24) maturation stages, respectively [17].

Flankers test

The Flanker test was employed in this study to evaluate selective visual attention and response inhibition, given its established reliability and appropriateness for the target population of young females [27]. Testing was conducted in a laboratory setting, with only one participant and the researcher present to maintain an undisturbed environment. To ensure a comfortable and conducive testing process, several adjustments were made: The chair height was adjusted to align the table with the participants’ elbow level, and if necessary, a box was placed under their feet to provide stable and supported foot positioning. The computer screen was positioned at an arm’s length from the participants, and the first row on the screen was adjusted to align with the participants’ eye height.

Participants were instructed to sit comfortably and position their left index finger on the key “A” and their right index finger on the key “L,” awaiting the commencement of the test. Subsequently, standardized instructions were provided to each participant. The target stimulus was consisted of one of four letters. The letters X and C had to be responded by hitting the A button of the keyboard, while the letters V and B had to be responded to with the L button. The response was dictated by the central letter alone, while ignoring the surrounded letters, which could be either congruent or incongruent. A practice trial was performed prior to the actual test and continued until the participant completed ten correct trials (correctly pressed the appropriate key). Following the practice trial, headphones were placed on the participant to minimize any potential disruptions caused by external noise sources. To minimize any potential observer effects, the examiner positioned themselves out of the participant’s visual field.

Statistical analysis

Four dependent variables were defined for the main analysis: Congruent / incongruent RT and success rate. The latter was calculated as the percentage of correct trials from all trials in the corresponding condition (i.e., inversely reflecting error rate). Consequently, flanker interference effect was calculated for each participant, for both RT and success rates. This was done by subtracting the mean congruent values from the mean incongruent values.

Data are presented using arithmetic mean and standard deviation. The Shapiro-Wilk tests and histograms were applied on each dependent variable to assess data distribution. Data was further tested for extreme outliers using boxplot with 3X interquartile range rejection criterion [28]. Incidentally, one participant (post-pubertal group) was excluded from the analysis for having significantly high RT values. For the main analysis (done separately for congruent and incongruent conditions), one-way analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) were employed, with four-level between group factor (PRE, EPUB, MPUB and LPUB) and corresponding success-rate as covariate. A similar analysis but without a covariate was performed for interference effect. We applied Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons. Effect-size estimates were assessed using ηp2, interpreted as: small 0.10–0.29, medium 0.30–0.49 and large ≥ 0.50 [29]. Success rates were also analysed independently for group differences using Kruskal-Wallis H test.

In addition, Pearson / Spearman correlation tests were performed between participant age and the dependent variables. This was done both on the entire group as well as withing groups. Correlation sizes were interpreted as negligible (0-0.3), low (0.3–0.5), moderate (0.5–0.7), high (0.7–0.9) or very high (0.9-1) [30]. A sensitivity power analysis was conducted using G*Power version 3.1.9.7 [25]. The analysis was based on Pearson’s correlation coefficient, with a statistical significance level of α = 0.05, power of 1-β = 0.80, and a sample size of n = 56. The outcome revealed a minimal detectable value for the correlation coefficient of Rp = 0.370.

All statistical analyses were performed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (version 28, IBM SPSS Statistics, Armonk, New York, USA) with significance level of α ≤ 0.05 set a-priory.

Results

Participant demographics and background information are presented in Table 1.

Table 1 Participants background information presented as mean (standard deviation)

Maturation effect

Significant maturation effect was observed for both congruent (F [4, 47] = 13.479, P ≤ 0.001, ηp2 = 0.534) and incongruent (F [4, 47] = 7.543, P ≤ 0.001 ηp2 = 0.391) reaction times (Fig. 1). Pairwise comparisons revealed significantly lower RTs (both congruent and incongruent) for the LPUB compared to PRE and EPUB groups (∆mean = 199-230ms, P < 0.002; ∆mean = 200-213ms P < 0.03, for PRE and EPUB respectively). Significantly shorter congruent RTs were also observed for MPUB compared to PRE (∆mean = 155ms, P = 0.010) and to EPUB (∆mean = 156ms, P = 0.017). While success rate, as covariate, significantly influenced the congruent RT comparison (p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.534), as an independent variable, there were no group effects. There were also no significant group differences in RT or success rate interference effects.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Reaction time comparison across maturation.(A) Scatter plot of congruent and incongruent reaction time (RT) for each maturation group.(B) Mean RT values. A clear improvement can be observed with maturation, both in congruent and incongruent RT values. Abbreviations: PRE, pre-pubertal; EPUB, early pubertal; MPUB, mid-pubertal; LPUB, late pubertal

Age effect

Moderate negative correlations were observed between age and reaction times (Rp = -0.695, P ≤ 0.001 for congruent and Rp = -0.614, P ≤ 0.001 for incongruent RT; Fig. 2). Age was not associated with flanker interference effects (both for RT and success rate) and had low positive correlation with incongruent success rates (Rs = 0.318, P = 0.022). In a further analysis, age correlations within each subgroup were tested. Only in the LPUB group, significant low to moderate negative correlations were observed between age and RT (Rs = -0.431, P = 0.035 for congruent and Rs = -0.532, P = 0.007 for incongruent RT) and between age and success rate (Rs = -0.574, P = 0.003 for congruent and Rs = 0.417, P = 0.043 for incongruent success rate).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Correlation analysis between age with Flankers test outcome variables. (A-B) Moderate negative correlations were observed for reaction times (RT); (C-D) Note: low positive correlation coefficient seen for congruent success rate, did not pass the sensitivity threshold

Discussion

To our knowledge, this was the first study to evaluate the effect of maturation status, rather than chronological age, on attentional control and response inhibition. Our results demonstrate a clear positive effect of maturation on choice reaction times, both congruent and incongruent, which were shorter in late maturation compared to early and pre-maturation. A similar relationship was observed for chronological age, with negative moderate correlations between age and reaction times. Congruent success rate was positively correlated with age, although not significantly different between maturation groups. Neither maturation level nor age were shown to affect flankers’ interference.

Shorter reaction times observed in LPUB participants may suggest a pivotal role of pubertal development in influencing cognitive performance, which tends to culminate towards full puberty. This finding is supported by Luna et al. [16], who demonstrated that processing speed and voluntary response suppression reach adulthood level at around ages of 14–15. In the current study, the mean age for the LPUB group was 14.1, although lower aged participants (12.6) were also classified as late pubertal stage according to maturity status and incidentally had lower RTs compared to the older participants in the group. In fact, correlations with age within the LPUB group were lower compared to the entire cohort age correlations. There was also an absence of correlations between age and reaction times in less mature groups, while LPUB success rate correlations were mixed, with negative correlations for congruent rates and positive correlations for incongruent. Together, these findings suggest that age alone may not be used to fully explain the measured constructs, though this practice is common [16, 31, 32]. In the current study, we used the Khamis-Roche method which has been found to provide a superior precision in maturity estimation compared to other methods which utilise various equations [18] Practically, such estimation is simple to apply by practitioners among adolescents, as it does not rely on direct measures such as skeletal age [33], dental maturity [34] or secondary sex characteristics [35]. Our findings carry important implications for practitioners working with adolescent athletes. Matching team assignments and tailoring training programs is currently based on chronological age. We propose, however, that maturation status should be taken into consideration when cognitive abilities are a requirement for a particular sport. Late-maturing individuals, despite their age, may face challenges in tasks whereas choice reaction is a major component (e.g., selecting where to pass a ball during a game), suggesting a potential disadvantage that warrants specific attention in training sessions [36].

While a positive correlation of incongruent success rate with age contrasted with the absence of significant differences for this outcome measure in relation to maturation subgroups, the observed correlation of 0.318 was below the minimal detectable value based on our sensitivity analysis. This strengthens the notion that success rate as an outcome might not be related to maturation stage nor to age. Consequently, it may suggest that participants prioritised accuracy over speed, resulting in similar accuracy rates between maturation subgroups but varying reaction times. It can also mean that this particular outcome is not sensitive enough with a clear ceiling effect that is achieved in relatively early development. It has previously been suggested that these two outcomes might benefit from a joint analysis rather than processed independently from one another [37]. Indeed, success rate accounted for some of the variability for congruent RT group difference, though not incongruent.

In the current study, neither maturation nor age were seen to be related to the flanker interference. It is likely that focusing on a central letter which is morphologically distinct than the alternative counts as a low perceptual task. Therefore, a high interference effect that was observed regardless of age might suggest that participants were potentially equally prone to distractibility [38]. While visual information processing time seemed to be different between groups, the interference effect is an individually normalised differential between congruent and incongruent RTs. Therefore, distractibility and, by extension, the interference effect could depend primarily on the perceptual load which did not exceed the capacity of even the pre-maturation participants. It is noteworthy that the Flankers test used in the current study included an element of working memory and thus, cognitive control. In this sense, a high cognitive, rather than perceptual, load was a staple of test performance. According to the load theory of attention [38], tasks with low perceptual load would typically result in greater distractibility compared to tasks that involve a high perceptual load. This is because when processing a task-relevant stimulus that is considerably high and closer to the individual’s perceptual capacity limits, processing distractors would theoretically result in exceeding this capacity [38].

Incidentally, caution is warranted when interpreting the findings of this study regarding the influence of puberty on inhibitory control. Despite the absence of any discernible effect of puberty on resistance to distractor interference in the Flanker test, puberty did exhibit an influence on selective attention, evident in reaction times when compatible and incompatible flankers were presented. It is essential to distinguish between resistance to distractor interference and inhibition. The former refers to the ability to resist or resolve interference from irrelevant external information, while the latter implies a specific mechanism or explanation for that effect [39,40,41]. Thus, the complexity of the interference effects suggests that other cognitive processes may be involved beyond straightforward inhibitory mechanisms [41]. Moreover, assessments such as the Flanker test, stop-signal task [42] or Stroop task [43] do not exclusively measure inhibition, as different task demands for each test may influence the shared features or patterns that can be linked to inhibitory processes [40, 44]. Additionally, it is worth noting that the inhibitory requirements for these repeated tasks may undergo changes upon task repetition, as highlighted in previous studies [41, 45]. Therefore, future research should carefully consider the specific features of inhibitory tests when attempting to assess the association between puberty and inhibitory control.

On another level, the question remains whether Flanker test performance is reflective of real-life sport situations that involve high perceptual load due to abundant peripheral stimuli. In these open skill situations, athletes interact with opponents as well as collaborate with their peers [46]. While evidence exists that sporting performance may be influenced by an athlete’s ability to supress their responses [46], the relationship between inhibitory control and sports performance is complex. Furthermore, rather than just ignoring irrelevant stimuli, spatial covert attention as a construct involves attending to peripheral information during complex tasks, not merely blocking it. We propose that even simple choice RT tasks with a response suppression element are to some extent a piece of the puzzle when investigating attentional control during real-life scenarios. Once more, this is particularly relevant for open-skill athletes, who have shown to demonstrate superior inhibitory control compared to closed-skill athletes [47]. In designing training routines for young athletes, it is crucial for coaches and sport practitioners to take into account the maturation level of the athletes. Specifically, practitioners should recognize that regardless of chronological age, less matured athletes may have different attentional capacities, adjust training protocols accordingly and align them with the athletes’ developmental neurocognitive capabilities. Although our cohort consisted of non-injured individuals, our findings do have potential implications for injury prevention, given that a majority of non-contact ACL injuries occur in situations that require fast decision making [9, 11]. Previous work has shown decreased neurocognitive performance among athletes that had suffered non-contact ACL injury compared to non-injured controls [48]. Although the risk of non-contact injuries is indeed significant during team sports [1], the latter evidence does not necessarily indicate a causal relationship, since various deficits might relate to post-injury neuroplasticity [49]. While our cohort was not consisted of injured persons, they have been active in team sports. In a recent work, Gonçalve et al. [50] demonstrated a positive association between maturation level (determined using the Khamis-Roche method) and peripheral perception among young soccer players, which was suggested to translate to superior efficiency in perceiving the complex environment of real-life game situations and subsequently, to better tactical performance.

Our study has several limitations, one of which is the uneven group sizes. The power analysis conducted prior to the study was based on the assumption of evenly distributed groups. The actual distribution thus led to a reduction of statistical power in the less matured groups. We argue, however, that smaller samples might suffice for early maturation groups given less variability in neurocognitive function within this demographic. Moreover, the most significant differences in RT were observed at late maturation, aligning with existing literature [16]. Furthermore, we initiated our study with a prior calculation using a high beta (1-β = 0.95) and recruited more participants than initially required. A post hoc power analysis using G*Power version 3.1.9.7 [23] and our results revealed that the achieved power was 1-β = 0.89 for the maturation effect on congruent RT and 1-β = 0.61 for maturation effect on incongruent RT, which seemed sufficient. Still, future studies are encouraged to confirm our findings by including larger cohorts in earlier maturation stages as well. Secondly, due to inherent variability between individuals, neither age nor maturation can be attributed as the only determinants of long/short reaction times. In that sense, longitudinal study designs are warranted to further establish the effect of maturation on neurocognitive function. Thirdly, we did not account for the potential impact of years of open-skill training on executive function in our older participants. While this trend has been systematically observed in typical children, particularly in the domain of working memory [51], no such influence was reported regarding inhibitory control. This suggests a minimal effect on the current findings. Finally, our cohort consisted of non-injured adolescents. Consequently, any inferences regarding the risk of non-contact injuries remain speculative. Future work should incorporate longitudinal follow-up documentation of potential injuries, to enhance our understanding of the risk and its relation to neurocognitive performance.

Conclusion

Our results demonstrate that maturation stage has a clear effect on visual information processing and selective attention among female adolescents active in team sports. In contrast, response inhibition did not seem to be affected by maturation level. These tendencies were comparable to an age effect, although age alone did not correlate with test performance within all maturation groups. Sport practitioners are encouraged to consider maturation level when developing training and injury prevention protocols among young athletes. Such practice could facilitate an individualised approach that is more compatible with their developmental neurocognitive capabilities.

Data availability

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

ACL:

anterior cruciate ligament

ANCOVA:

analysis of covariance

ANOVA:

analysis of variance

EPUB:

early pubertal group

LPUB:

late pubertal group

MPUB:

mid-pubertal group

PRE:

pre-pubertal group

RT:

reaction time

References

  1. Chia L, De Oliveira Silva D, Whalan M, McKay MJ, Sullivan J, Fuller CW, Pappas E. Non-contact anterior cruciate ligament injury epidemiology in team-ball sports: a systematic review with meta-analysis by sex, age, sport, participation level, and exposure type. Sports Med 2022:1–21.

  2. Montalvo AM, Schneider DK, Yut L, Webster KE, Beynnon B, Kocher MS, Myer GD. What’s my risk of sustaining an ACL injury while playing sports? A systematic review with meta-analysis. Br J Sports Med. 2018.

  3. Montalvo AM, Schneider DK, Webster KE, Yut L, Galloway MT, Heidt RS Jr, Kaeding CC, Kremcheck TE, Magnussen RA, Parikh SN. Anterior cruciate ligament injury risk in sport: a systematic review and meta-analysis of injury incidence by sex and sport classification. J Athl Train. 2019;54(5):472–82.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Bram JT, Magee LC, Mehta NN, Patel NM, Ganley TJ. Anterior cruciate ligament injury incidence in adolescent athletes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Sports Med. 2021;49(7):1962–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Hughes G. A review of recent perspectives on biomechanical risk factors associated with anterior cruciate ligament injury. Res Sports Med. 2014;22(2):193–212.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Boden BP, Torg JS, Knowles SB, Hewett TE. Video analysis of anterior cruciate ligament injury: abnormalities in hip and ankle kinematics. Am J Sports Med. 2009;37(2):252–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Koga H, Nakamae A, Shima Y, Iwasa J, Myklebust G, Engebretsen L, Bahr R, Krosshaug T. Mechanisms for noncontact anterior cruciate ligament injuries: knee joint kinematics in 10 injury situations from female team handball and basketball. Am J Sports Med. 2010;38(11):2218–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Krosshaug T, Nakamae A, Boden BP, Engebretsen L, Smith G, Slauterbeck JR, Hewett TE. Bahr RJTAjosm: mechanisms of anterior cruciate ligament injury in basketball: video analysis of 39 cases. Am J Spots Med. 2007, 35(3):359–67.

  9. Della Villa F, Buckthorpe M, Grassi A, Nabiuzzi A, Tosarelli F, Zaffagnini S, Della Villa S. Systematic video analysis of ACL injuries in professional male football (soccer): injury mechanisms, situational patterns and biomechanics study on 134 consecutive cases. Br J Sports Med. 2020;54(23):1423–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Waldén M, Krosshaug T, Bjørneboe J, Andersen TE, Faul O, Hägglund M. Three distinct mechanisms predominate in non-contact anterior cruciate ligament injuries in male professional football players: a systematic video analysis of 39 cases. Br J Sports Med. 2015;49(22):1452–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Hughes G, Dai B. The influence of decision making and divided attention on lower limb biomechanics associated with anterior cruciate ligament injury: a narrative review. Sports Biomech 2021:1–16.

  12. The Kinetic Chain Has a Brain: Could Adding Cognitive Demands in Lower Limb Injury Prevention Programs Improve Outcomes? In., vol. 53: JOSPT, Inc. JOSPT, 1020 North Fairfax Street, Suite 400-E, Alexandria,VA … 9–161.

  13. Swanik CB. Brains and sprains: the brain’s role in noncontact anterior cruciate ligament injuries. J Athl Train. 2015;50(10):1100–2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Desimone R, Duncan J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1995;18(1):193–222.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Korkman M, Kemp SL, Kirk U. Effects of age on neurocognitive measures of children ages 5 to 12: a cross-sectional study on 800 children from the United States. Dev Neuropsychol. 2001;20(1):331–54.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Luna B, Garver KE, Urban TA, Lazar NA, Sweeney JA. Maturation of cognitive processes from late childhood to adulthood. Child Dev. 2004;75(5):1357–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Cumming SP, Lloyd RS, Oliver JL, Eisenmann JC, Malina RM. Bio-banding in sport: applications to competition, talent identification, and strength and conditioning of youth athletes. Strength Conditioning J. 2017;39(2):34–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Towlson C, Salter J, Ade JD, Enright K, Harper LD, Page RM, Malone JJ. Maturity-associated considerations for training load, injury risk, and physical performance in youth soccer: one size does not fit all. J Sport Health Sci. 2021;10(4):403–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Backes EP, Bonnie RJ, National Academies of Sciences E, Medicine. Adolescent development. The Promise of Adolescence: realizing opportunity for all Youth. edn.: National Academies Press (US); 2019.

  20. Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept Psychophys. 1974;16(1):143–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Clayson PE, Larson MJ. Psychometric properties of conflict monitoring and conflict adaptation indices: response time and conflict N 2 event-related potentials. Psychophysiology. 2013;50(12):1209–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Huizinga M, Dolan CV, Van der Molen MW. Age-related change in executive function: developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(11):2017–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Ridderinkhof KR, van der Molen MW. A psychophysiological analysis of developmental differences in the ability to resist interference. Child Dev. 1995;66(4):1040–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Khamis HJ, Roche AF. Predicting adult stature without using skeletal age: the Khamis-Roche method. Pediatrics. 1994;94(4):504–7.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G* power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39(2):175–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Ladouceur CD, Dahl RE, Carter CS. ERP correlates of action monitoring in adolescence. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1021(1):329–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Rueda MR, Fan J, McCandliss BD, Halparin JD, Gruber DB, Lercari LP, Posner MI. Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42(8):1029–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Iglewicz B, Hoaglin DC. How to detect and handle outliers. Quality; 1993.

  29. Richardson JTE. Eta squared and partial eta squared as measures of effect size in educational research. Educ Res Rev-Neth. 2011;6(2):135–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Mukaka MM. A guide to appropriate use of correlation coefficient in medical research. Malawi Med J. 2012;24(3):69–71.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Cromer JA, Schembri AJ, Harel BT, Maruff P. The nature and rate of cognitive maturation from late childhood to adulthood. Front Psychol. 2015;6:704.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Tamm L, Menon V, Reiss AL. Maturation of brain function associated with response inhibition. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41(10):1231–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Chumela WC, Roche AF, Thissen D. The FELS method of assessing the skeletal maturity of the hand-wrist. Am J Hum Biology. 1989;1(2):175–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Rai V, Saha S, Yadav G, Tripathi AM, Grover K. Dental and skeletal maturity-a biological indicator of chronologic age. J Clin Diagn Research: JCDR. 2014;8(9):ZC60.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Tanner JM. Growth at adolescence. 1962.

  36. Coker C. Motor learning and control for practitioners. Routledge; 2017.

  37. Salthouse TA. Is flanker-based inhibition related to age? Identifying specific influences of individual differences on neurocognitive variables. Brain Cogn. 2010;73(1):51–61.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Lavie N, Hirst A, De Fockert JW, Viding E. Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2004;133(3):339.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. MacLeod CM, Dodd MD, Sheard ED, Wilson DE, Bibi U. In opposition to inhibition. Psychol Learn Motivation. 2003;43:163–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex frontal lobe tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol. 2000;41(1):49–100.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Friedman NP, Miyake A. The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2004;133(1):101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Logan GD. On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A users’ guide to the stop signal paradigm. 1994.

  43. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. 1935;18(6):643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Shilling V, Chetwynd A, Rabbitt P. Individual inconsistency across measures of inhibition: an investigation of the construct validity of inhibition in older adults. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40(6):605–19.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Gärtner A, Strobel A. Individual differences in inhibitory control: a latent variable analysis. J Cognition 2021, 4(1).

  46. Albaladejo-García C, García-Aguilar F, Moreno FJ. The role of inhibitory control in sport performance: systematic review and meta-analysis in stop-signal paradigm. Neurosci Biobehavioral Reviews 2023:105108.

  47. Formenti D, Trecroci A, Duca M, Cavaggioni L, D’Angelo F, Passi A, Longo S, Alberti G. Differences in inhibitory control and motor fitness in children practicing open and closed skill sports. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):4033.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Swanik CB, Covassin T, Stearne DJ, Schatz P. The relationship between neurocognitive function and noncontact anterior cruciate ligament injuries. Am J Sports Med. 2007;35(6):943–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Grooms D, Appelbaum G, Onate J. Neuroplasticity following anterior cruciate ligament injury: a framework for visual-motor training approaches in rehabilitation. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther. 2015;45(5):381–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Gonçalves E, Noce F, Barbosa MAM, Figueiredo AJ, Hackfort D, Teoldo I. Correlation of the peripheral perception with the maturation and the effect of the peripheral perception on the tactical behaviour of soccer players. Int J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2020;18(5):687–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Feng X, Zhang Z, Jin T, Shi P. Effects of open and closed skill exercise interventions on executive function in typical children: a meta-analysis. BMC Psychol. 2023;11(1):420.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank the female players from the volleyball academy VAM Olomouc, Czech Republic, for participating in this study. Additionally, we thank the coaches for dedicating training time and assisting with organization. Finally, special thanks to Richard Sylvester and Karolína Válová for their help with data collection.

Funding

No external funding was received for this study.

Open access funding provided by Umea University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

AG designed and performed the main analysis, interpreted the results, drafted and revised the manuscript. IH contributed to the conceptualisation and methodology of the study, collected the data and contributed to the writing of the manuscript. ML contributed to the conceptualisation of the study, recruited participants and contributed to the writing of the manuscript. RA interpreted the results and contributed to the writing of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Adam Grinberg.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Human ethics and consent to participate

The study received approval from the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Physical Culture, Palacký University Olomouc (reference number: 15/2023) and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent documents were obtained from all guardians of the participants, after providing them with detailed information about the potential risks associated with the testing

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Grinberg, A., Hanzlíková, I., Lehnert, M. et al. The impact of maturation level, not chronological age, on attentional control: implications for sports injury prevention in female adolescents. BMC Sports Sci Med Rehabil 16, 195 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13102-024-00984-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13102-024-00984-5

Keywords