Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Combining perceptual regulation and exergaming for exercise prescription in low-active adults with and without cognitive impairment

Abstract

 Background

Exercise adherence in already low-active older adults with and without mild cognitive impairment (MCI) remains low. Perceptual regulation and exergaming may facilitate future exercise behaviour by improving the affective experience, however evidence that this population can perceptually regulate is lacking. To explore this, we investigated 1) perceptual regulation of exercise intensity during either exergaming or regular ergometer cycling and 2) explored affective responses.

Methods

Thirty-two low active older adults (73.9 ± 7.3 years, n = 16, 8 females) with or without MCI (70.9 ± 5.5 years, n = 16, 11 females) participated in a sub-maximal fitness assessment to determine ventilatory threshold (VT) and two experimental sessions (counterbalanced: exergaming or regular ergometer cycling). Experimental sessions consisted 21-min of continuous cycling with 7-min at each: RPE 9, 11 and 13. Oxygen consumption (VO2), heart rate (HR), and affect (Feeling Scale) were obtained throughout the exercise.

Results

VO2 (p < 0.01) and HR (p < 0.01) increased linearly with RPE, but were not significantly different between exercise modes or cognitive groups. At RPE 13, participants worked above VT in both modes (exergaming: 115.7 ± 27.3; non-exergaming 114.1 ± 24.3 VO2 (%VT)). Regardless of cognitive group, affect declined significantly as RPE increased (p < 0.01). However on average, affect remained pleasant throughout and did not differ between exercise modes or cognitive groups.

Conclusions

These results suggest low-active older adults can perceptually regulate exercise intensity, regardless of cognition or mode. At RPE 13, participants regulated above VT, at an intensity that improves cardiorespiratory fitness long-term, and affect remained positive in the majority of participants, which may support long-term physical activity adherence.

Peer Review reports

Background

Australian physical activity (PA) guidelines indicate that as few as one in three older men and one in five older women (> 65 years) are considered sufficiently active (achieving at least 150 min moderate intensity activity per week) [1, 2]. In this population, regular PA may not only be preventative against cognitive decline and reduce the risk of future cognitive impairment [3,4,5], but might also slow the progression of an existing mild cognitive impairment (MCI) to dementia [6,7,8]. Despite this, one of the biggest challenges is engaging low-active older adults, both with and without cognitive impairment, in regular and sustainable PA. Evidence from one study demonstrates only 53% of older adults living with MCI adhere to a six month traditionally prescribed, moderate intensity exercise program; and as few as 25% maintain sufficient PA, six months after completion [9]. Taken together, these findings suggest traditional, moderate intensity exercise prescription approaches do not translate to long-term adherence, in this population. Therefore, new and novel exercise prescription approaches are needed to combat both physical inactivity and age-related cognitive decline.

One method of exercise prescription increasingly recognised as an effective way to promote long-term adherence is perceptual regulation of the exercise intensity [10]. Rather than an externally controlled intensity, such as percentage of age-predicted maximal heart rate (%HRmax), participants use effort perception (with the Borg 6–20 Rating of Perceived Exertion (RPE) Scale) and their internal framework to individually set the intensity [11]. At any point throughout the exercise, participants can increase or decrease their output to maintain the desired effort perception. When perceptually regulating at different RPEs, participants work at discretely different intensities with high repeatability and reliability [10, 12, 13]. Additionally, the process of perceptual regulation has been linked to increased exercise adherence [10]. It is likely, that the autonomy present when perceptually regulating, and the acute affective (pleasant/unpleasant) responses during exercise, underpin the increased adherence [14]. A growing body of evidence shows promise for the use of perceptual regulation in sedentary adults [15], clinical populations [16, 17] and sufficiently active older adults [18]. Preliminary evidence in active older adults with no cardiovascular disease risk factors other than age, supports the use of RPE and perceptual regulation during a fitness assessment to accurately predict cardiorespiratory fitness [18]. However, there is no current evidence for the use of RPE to regulate exercise intensity in low-active older adults, or those living with MCI, and indeed many clinicians and care workers indicate at least anecdotally, that low-active older adults and people with MCI do not understand how to regulate their exercise intensity using the RPE scale.

Another emerging exercise modality that may support long-term adherence is exergaming [19]. Exergaming combines physical exercise with a computer-simulated interactive game. Studies report greater improvements in cognitive outcomes (including executive function) occur following a three month exergaming intervention, compared to traditionally prescribed exercise in adults with MCI [20]. Interestingly participants who used exergaming also anecdotally reported increased enjoyment during exercise [20], which may support future adherence to the intervention. A systematic review of the literature further demonstrates increased physical and cognitive benefits of exergaming in older adults, compared to traditional exercise interventions [21]. Together these studies provide support for exergaming as an effective approach for exercise prescription in older low-active adults, with and without MCI.

Two models may partially explain the increased enjoyment with the use of exergaming: Ekkekakis’s [22] dual-mode model and Tenenbaum’s social cognitive theory of perceived and sustained effort [23]. Whilst not the same, both models propose a link between awareness of internal (physiological) sensations during exercise and affective responses, with a negative shift as the intensity increases. The dual-mode model [22], proposes affective responses are largely guided by cognitive processes and are uniformly positive at low intensities [22, 24]. However, as the intensity of exercise increases beyond a point of physiological steady state (defined as the ventilatory threshold (VT)), physiological cues dominate, and this is associated with a homogenous unpleasant response [25, 26]. Exergaming, at intensities around VT, may distract from unpleasant (physiological) sensations and lead to more pleasant affective responses and higher exercise outputs, particularly if perceptually regulating.

Therefore this study had two overarching aims. Firstly, we aimed to investigate if low-active older adults with and without MCI were able to perceptually-regulate their exercise intensity at three submaximal intensities (relative to RPE 9-very light, RPE 11-light and RPE 13-somewhat hard) during exergaming or non-exergaming. We hypothesised 1) that older adults, regardless of their cognitive ability, will be able to perceptually regulate exercise at the three intensities. and 2) exergaming mode will be associated with higher work rates compared to standard ergometer cycling. Similar to studies in other populations [10, 26], we also hypothesised 3) that work rate selected at RPE 13 will be around known physiological thresholds (VT);

Additionally, we aimed to investigate affective responses across the duration of the exercise sessions. We hypothesised 4) that affective responses will remain pleasant across each submaximal intensity but less at RPE 13 compared to RPE 9 and 11 and 5) the exergaming condition will be associated with more pleasant affective responses at each intensity compared to non-exergaming.

These findings will provide the first evidence for the combined use of perceptual regulation and exergaming in this at-risk population.

Method

Participants

Thirty-two insufficiently active older adults provided informed written consent and participated in the study (Table 1). Eligibility for participation was determined by self-reporting less than 150 min of moderate intensity PA per week [1]. Recruitment occurred through local newspaper advertisements targeting low-active older adults who were concerned about their memory. All experimental procedures were approved by the University of South Australia’s human research ethics committee and were performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki.

Table 1 Participant characteristics

Scores from the Addenbrooke Cognitive Exam (ACE-III) were used to dichotomise participants into groups using a previously reported clinical cut-off [27,28,29] (apparently healthy (score ≥ 88/100) or MCI (score < 88)).

Experimental protocol

Participants attended the laboratory on three separate occasions within a four-week period (once for a screening session and twice for the experimental sessions).

Screening session: Session 1

Initially, height, mass, percent body fat (Tanita BF-679 W bioelectric impedance analysis scale; Tanita Corporation, Tokyo, Japan), blood pressure and resting heart rate (Dinamap Pro 100 automated sphygmomanometer), fasted total and high density cholesterol, and blood glucose (CardioCheck PA Point-of-Care Device, Indianapolis, IN) were measured. Participants were then served a standardised breakfast of toast and cereal.

Following familiarisation with the Borg 6 to 20 Scale and the Feeling Scale (used to assess affective responses), participants completed an experimenter-controlled submaximal exercise test on a recumbent ergometer (Lode Corival Recumbent, Groningen, The Netherlands) to assess cardiorespiratory fitness and determine VT.

Oxygen uptake (VO2) was measured continuously via a breath-by-breath automatic gas exchange system (Cortex MetaAnalyzer 3B, Biophysik, and Cortex Metasoft 3.1 software, Leipzig, Germany). Heart rate was continuously monitored using a wireless chest strap telemetry system (Polar Electro T31, Kempele, Finland).

The exercise test began with a 2-min warm up and familiarisation. Participants were instructed to cycle between 60 and 70 rpm. The test began at 15 W or 25 W and increased in either 15 W or 25 W increments each minute. The minute-by-minute incremental increases in resistance (either 15 W or 25 W) were determined by the sex of the participants and self-perceived fitness levels, with the aim of achieving test completion between 5 and 12 min of exercise. Individual RPE and affect were obtained each minute, and the test was terminated when participants reported an RPE of 15.

Experimental sessions: Session 2 and 3:

The order of the experimental sessions were randomised and counterbalanced so half of the participants received the exergaming session first and half the non-exergaming.

Both the Feeling Scale and the Borg 6–20 RPE Scale were displayed in front of the participants. Participants were asked to report affect and RPE prior to exercise, every two minutes for the entire duration of the test and immediately following exercise completion. Heart rate and VO2 were recoded continuously throughout the duration of the exercise session.

Session two and three, commenced with a brief 2-min warm up on the Expresso HD Recumbent Bike. Participants were then instructed to cycle for 21 continuous minutes, consisting of seven minutes at RPE 9 (equivalent to a ‘very light’ intensity), seven minutes at RPE 11 (equivalent to a ‘light’ intensity) and seven minutes at RPE 13 (equivalent to a ‘some-what hard’ intensity) either using the ‘Track mode’ of the Expresso bike (exergaming) or with the screen blocked (non-exergaming).

The exergaming session required the participants to cycle and steer their “avatar” around a pre-defined track steering with the steering levers attached to the side of the seat, while maintaining the targeted exercise output. During the control session the screen was blocked out and participants were not required to steer, but the same cycle ergometer was used. All outputs were concealed from the participants during the testing.

Throughout each exercise session, participants were reminded they could change resistance, or cycling cadence at any point to maintain the required exercise intensity. A continuous 21-min exercise duration was chosen so that seven minutes could be spent at each intensity, which allowed for multiple measurements at each intensity and replicated a standard aerobic session.

Prediction of fitness and determination of ventilatory threshold

To predict cardiorespiratory fitness levels, VO2 was collated from the final 30 s of each minute from the submaximal exercise test and extrapolated with HR using linear regression to an age-predicted HR max (obtained from Tanaka [30] equation, 208–0.7*age). The VT was individually determined using a triangulation of the modified v-slope, ventilatory equivalents (VE) and excess CO2 methods previously described by Gaskill and colleagues [31]. For the v-slope method, VT was identified as the point at which there was a disproportionate increase in VCO2 compared to VO2. For the VE method, VT was identified as the exercise intensity corresponding to the first sustained disproportionate increase in VE/VO2 with no increase in VE/VCO2. For the excess CO2 method, VT was identified as the exercise intensity corresponding to an increase in excess CO2 from steady state. The three methods were graphed separately for each participant and VT identified by two independent raters (LM and AS). If there was a disagreement on the location of the VT, the data were independently assessed by a third rater (GP or CG). For all subsequent analyses, VO2 was expressed relative to VT and all HR data as a percentage of age-predicted HRmax [30].

Statistical analysis

To investigate if older low-active adults could regulate exercise intensity using perceptual regulation (hypothesis 1 and 2) physiological variables (VO2 and HR) were analysed with separate three-way analysis of variances (ANOVAs). Within subject factors were mode (2 levels: exergaming or non-exergaming) and intensity (3 levels: RPE 9, 11 and 13). The between subjects factor was cognitive group (2 levels: Apparently healthy or MCI). To ascertain if work rate at RPE 13 was around VT (hypothesis 3) a one-way ANOVA was conducted to compare VO2 at VT from the submaximal graded exercise test to the average of VO2 during the final minute of RPE 13 (minute 20) of both the exergaming and non-exergaming modes. To explore the stabilisation of physiological responses (VO2 and HR) within each of the three submaximal intensities, the coefficient of variations were obtained for each intensity by averaging the VO2 (last 30 s) in two-minute blocks at each exercise intensity and then obtaining a ratio of standard deviation to the mean, expressed as a percentage. Coefficient of variations were also analysed with three-way ANOVA. Within subject factors were mode (2 levels: exergaming or non-exergaming) and intensity (3 levels: RPE 9, 11 and 13) and the between subjects factor was cognitive group (2 levels: Apparently healthy or MCI).

Affective responses (Feeling scale) were also analysed with a three-way ANOVA (hypothesis 4 and 5). Within subject factors were mode (2 levels: exergaming or non-exergaming) and intensity (3 levels: RPE 9, 11 and 13). The between subjects factor was cognitive group (2 levels: Apparently healthy or MCI).

Post Hoc tests with Bonferroni correction were conducted on all significant main effects or interactions. Normal distribution and homogeneity of variance of the data were assessed using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test and Levene’s statistic, respectively. Effect sizes are presented as Eta squared (η2) [32]. In ANOVAs where assumptions of sphericity were violated, the critical value of F was adjusted using the Greenhouse-Geisser epsilon value. All data were analysed using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) version 22 software.

Results

Thirty-one participants completed both non-exergaming and exergaming sessions, while one participant (apparently healthy group) was excluded for changes to their medication during the study. Three participants (1 apparently healthy, 2 MCI) did not have valid fitness data to predict VT or fitness, and this is reflected in the difference in the degrees of freedom for specific analyses. Sixteen participants scored equal to or below the clinical threshold of 88/100 on the ACE-III.

Table 1, illustrates the mean descriptive participant characteristics between the cognitive groups. There were no differences in age, predicted cardiorespiratory fitness or VT. However, as expected ACE-III score was lower in the MCI group (t [30]= − 7.984, P < 0.001).

Physiological evidence consistent with the use of perceptual regulation

In support of hypothesis 1, there was a step-wise increase in VO2 at each RPE (F [2, 52] = 71.00, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.52; Fig. 1a and b) but no other main effects of mode (hypothesis 2), cognitive group or interactions with mode or cognitive group. Similarly, there was a step-wise increase in HR at each RPE (F [1.3, 40.18] = 94.27, P < 0.01, η2 = 0.55, Fig. 1c and d) and no other main effects or interactions.

Fig. 1
figure 1

VO2 (a and b) and HR data (c and d) during 21 min of non-exergaming (open symbols) and exergaming (coloured symbols) in the MCI group (a and c) and the apparently healthy group (b and d). VO2 (% VT) and HR (% HRmax) increased with each RPE level. There were no differences in either VO2 or HR between exercise modes (exergaming or non-exergaming). *P < 0.05, error bars indicate standard error of the mean (SEM)

Physiological evidence of working above ventilatory threshold at RPE 13

At RPE 13 (hypothesis 3), VO2 was significantly higher in the exergaming mode, compared to the VO2 at RPE 13 in the submaximal exercise test and the non-exergaming mode (F[2,54] = 4.10, P = 0.02, η2 = 0.13). There was no significant difference between VO2 at VT in minute 20 of the non-exergaming and the VO2 at VT in the submaximal exercise test (P = 0.14).

The coefficient of variation analysis of physiological variables (VO2 and HR) revealed no main effects for intensity, mode, cognitive group or interactions (Table 2).

Table 2 Coefficient of variation (%) of physiological variables

Affective responses during perceptual regulation

For affect (hypothesis 4 and 5) there was a main effect of intensity (F[2, 58] = 38.15, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.28, Fig. 2), but no other main effects or interactions for mode or cognitive group. Affect declined from RPE 9 to RPE 11 (P = 0.002) and RPE 11 to RPE 13 (P < 0.001). Despite this, the mean group affective responses remained pleasant each: RPE 9, RPE 11 and RPE 13 (Table 3). During the final time point of each RPE level, a small proportion of participants reported below neutral responses (Table 3).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Affective responses across the 21-min of exergaming (coloured symbols) or non-exergaming (open symbols) in either MCI group (a) or apparently healthy (b). Affect significantly declined, but on average remained positive throughout the entire session. There was no difference in affective responses between exercise modes (exergaming or non-exergaming). *P < 0.05 across RPE levels, the error bars indicate standard error of the mean (SEM)

Table 3 Affective responses during perceptual regulation, mean ± SD (% participants reporting negative affect)

Discussion

This study provides the first evidence that low-active older adults, regardless of MCI, are able to perceptually regulate their exercise intensity at three submaximal levels (RPE 9, 11 and 13). In line with other research [26], participants choose an intensity around their individual VT at RPE 11 and RPE 13, which can improve fitness long-term [10, 33]. Additionally, for the majority of participants, affective responses remained pleasant throughout. Unexpectedly, with the added stimulus of exergaming compared to non-exergaming we showed no differences in work rate or affect. Together these findings highlight the potential clinical benefit of using perceptual regulation in this at-risk population.

A future practical application of these findings for this population, might be an intervention using perceptual regulation anchored at RPE 11 or 13, to improve fitness. Indeed, in a similar population individualised training programs, externally prescribed at the VT were well tolerated, and led to greater improvements in fitness over a 12-week intervention, compared to traditional prescription at 50% HR reserve [17, 34]. A limitation of externally controlled individualised programs is the need to repeatedly assess/reassess the VT across the intervention, as participants improve fitness. Other studies, using perceptual regulation during interventions, albeit in younger populations, demonstrate participants regulate their output at higher work rates as they improve fitness (for the same RPE), thus negating the need to assess/reassess VT [10, 35].

A growing body of literature, demonstrates the importance of considering the affective response to exercise, alongside the intensity, to reduce exercise drop out [14, 36, 37]. In the present study, whilst affect consistently became less pleasant from RPE 9 to 11 and RPE 11 to 13, the majority of participants reported pleasant affective responses across the entire duration of the test. This characteristic shift in affect to become more unpleasant has been similarly reported in other populations when the intensity remains unchanged for a long duration [24, 38]. It is also important to note that towards the later stages of the sessions (RPE 13) 29% of participants reported an unpleasant affective response. One potential explanation for this response, is the long duration of the session (21 min) in a low-active population who do not exercise for extended periods, or additionally the higher work rate, which always occurred after 14 min of exercise at RPE 9 and 11. In light of these findings, we would be inclined to suggest it may be more appropriate at least initially, to perceptually regulate at RPE 11-light in this population. Anchoring exertion at a lower level would likely increase pleasant affective responses and reduce drop-out rates in an intervention, particularly in participants who report unpleasant affect at RPE 13. Future studies could also consider reordering the RPE levels, so participants finish the session at a lower RPE, which may uniformly be associated with a pleasant affect [39].

Unexpectedly, we saw no differences in work rate or affect in exergaming compared to non-exergaming. A potential explanation for this, is the immersion level of the particular game chosen provided insufficient distraction from internal physiological cues. Since there were few studies to base our design, a low level immersive option was pragmatically chosen to not over burden the participants cognitively. However, future studies using exergaming should consider immersion level. It is also possible that by asking participants to perceptually regulate, we may have inadvertently anchored the work rate, regardless of mode. Indeed, as reported in other studies, self-selection of exercise intensity [38], rather than perceptual regulation may be a more appropriate way to increase work rate and affect with exergaming.

Study limitations

There are a few other study limitations that warrant discussion. Firstly, it is important to note this was an exploratory study with a small sample size and the sample was intentionally collected from older adults with memory concerns. This may affect the generalisation of results to the wider population of low active older adults. To be included in the study, participants were required to self-report as low-active, not meeting current activity of 150 min MVPA per week. However, actual physical activity was not verified and only a measurement of cardiovascular fitness was conducted. Both cognitive groups were well matched for fitness (Table 1), and predicted VO2max values fell below the fiftieth percentile for women and the thirtieth percentile for men when compared to the Australian fitness normative values indicating participants were likely insufficiently active [40]. However, it is possible that differences in the ability to perceptually regulate and affective responses to exercise in extremely physically inactive individuals may still exist. A larger sample size of extremely insufficiently active participants would be required to assess this.

Conclusions

To combat growing physical inactivity, particularly in older adults with and without MCI, new and novel prescription approaches are needed [9]. Here, we provide the first evidence that low-active older adults (with and without MCI) can perceptually regulate their exercise intensity using the Borg 6–20 RPE scale and they do so at a similar intensity to other populations [12, 26, 41, 42]. Furthermore, the majority report a pleasant affective responses, which may support future adherence, long-term [14].

Abbreviations

%HRmax:

Percent heart rate maximum

ACE-III:

Addenbrooke Cognitive Exam (Version 3)

ANOVA:

Analysis of variance

CO2 :

Carbon dioxide

HR:

Heart rate

MCI:

Mild Cognitive Impairment

PA:

Physical activity

RPE:

Rating of perceived exertion

SPSS:

Statistical Package for Social Sciences

VE:

Ventilatory equivalent

VO2 :

Oxygen uptake

VT :

Ventilatory threshold

W:

Watts

References

  1. Commonwealth of Australia and the Repatriation Commission. In: DoVA, editor. Choose Health: Be Active: A physical activity guide for older Australians. Canberra: Department of Health and Aging; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Australian Bureau of Statistics (ABS). Australian Health Survey: Physical Activity 2011–12, cat. no. 4364.0.55.004. Canberra: ABS; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Virtuoso Júnior JS, Tribess S, Paulo TRSD, Martins CA, Romo-Perez V. Physical activity as an indicator of predictive functional disability in elderly. Rev Lat Am Enfermagem. 2012;20:259–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Manini TM, Pahor M. Physical activity and maintaining physical function in older adults. Br J Sports Med. 2009;43:28–31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Yaffe K, Barnes D, Nevitt M, Lui L-Y, Covinsky K. A prospective study of physical activity and cognitive decline in elderly women: women who walk. Arch Intern Med. 2001;161:1703–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Laurin D, Verreault R, Lindsay J, MacPherson K, Rockwood K. Physical activity and risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in elderly persons. Arch Neurol. 2001;58:498–504.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Hamer M, Chida Y. Physical activity and risk of neurodegenerative disease: a systematic review of prospective evidence. Psychol Med. 2009;39:3–11.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Valenzuela MJ, Sachdev P. Brain reserve and cognitive decline: a non-parametric systematic review. Psychol Med. 2006;36:1065–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Tak EC, van Uffelen JG, Paw MJ, van Mechelen W, Hopman-Rock M. Adherence to exercise programs and determinants of maintenance in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. J Aging Phys Act. 2012;20:32–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Parfitt G, Evans H, Eston RG. Perceptually regulated training at RPE13 is pleasant and improves physical health. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2012;44:1613–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Borg G. Borg's perceived exertion and pain scales. Champaign: Human Kinetics; 1998.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Eston RG, Evans H, Faulkner J, Lambrick D, Al-Rahamneh H, Parfitt G. A perceptually regulated, graded exercise test predicts peak oxygen uptake during treadmill exercise in active and sedentary participants. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2012;112:3459–68.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Coquart JB, Garcin M, Parfitt G, Tourny-Chollet C, Eston RG. Prediction of maximal or peak oxygen uptake from ratings of perceived exertion. Sports Med. 2014;44:563–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Williams D, Dunsiger S, Ciccolo JT, Lewis BA, Albrecht AE, Marcus BH. Acute affective response to a moderate-intensity exercise stimulus predicts physical activity participation 6 and 12 months later. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2008;9:231–45.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Focht BC, Knapp DJ, Gavin TP, Raedeke TD, Hickner RC. Affective and self-efficacy responses to acute aerobic exercise in sedentary older and younger adults. J Aging Phys Act. 2007;15:123–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Eston RG, Connolly D. The use of ratings of perceived exertion for exercise prescription in patients receiving β-blocker therapy. Sports Med. 1996;21:176–90.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Tang LH, Zwisler A-D, Taylor RS, Doherty P, Zangger G, Berg SK, Langberg H. Self-rating level of perceived exertion for guiding exercise intensity during a 12-week cardiac rehabilitation programme and the influence of heart rate reducing medication. J Sci Med Sport. 2016;19:611–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Smith AE, Eston RG, Norton PG. A perceptually-regulated exercise test predicts peak oxygen uptake in older active adults. J Aging Phys Act. 2015;23:205–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Leininger L, Coles M, Gilbert J. Comparing enjoyment and perceived exertion between equivalent bouts of physically interactive video gaming and treadmill walking. Health Fitness J Canada. 2010;3:12–8.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Anderson-Hanley C, Arciero PJ, Brickman AM, Nimon JP, Okuma N, Westen SC, Merz ME, Pence BD, Woods JA, Kramer AF, Zimmerman EA. Exergaming and older adult cognition: a cluster randomized clinical trial. Am J Prev Med. 2012;42:109–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Bleakley CM, Charles D, Porter-Armstrong A, McNeill MD, McDonough SM, McCormack B. Gaming for health a systematic review of the physical and cognitive effects of interactive computer games in older adults. J Aging Phys Act 2013;20:S238–9.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Ekkekakis P. Pleasure and displeasure from the body: perspectives from exercise. Cognit Emot. 2003;17:213–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Tenebaum G, Hutchinson JC: A social-cognitive perspective of perceived and sustained effort. In Handbook of Sport Psychology Volume 3. 3rd edition. Edited by Eklund RC, Tenenbaum G. Hoboken: Wiley; 2007: 560–578.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  24. Rose E, Parfitt G. A quantitative analysis and qualitative explanation of the individual differences in affective responses to prescribed and self-selected exercise intensities. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2007;29:281–309.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Parfitt G, Sheppard K. Patterning of physiological and affective responses during a graded exercise test in sedentary men and boys. J Exerc Sci Fit. 2008;6:121–9.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Smith AE, Eston RG, Tempest GD, Norton B, Parfitt G. Patterning of physiological and affective responses in older active adults during a maximal graded exercise test and self-selected exercise. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2015;115:1855–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Mioshi E, Dawson K, Mitchell J, Arnold R, Hodges JR. The Addenbrooke's cognitive examination revised (ACE-R): a brief cognitive test battery for dementia screening. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;21:1078–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Yoshida H, Terada S, Honda H, Kishimoto Y, Takeda N, Oshima E, Hirayama K, Yokota O, Uchitomi Y. Validation of the revised Addenbrooke's cognitive examination (ACE-R) for detecting mild cognitive impairment and dementia in a Japanese population. Int Psychogeriatr. 2012;24:28–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. McColgan P, Evans JR, Breen DP, Mason SL, Barker RA, Williams-Gray CH. Addenbrooke's cognitive examination-revised for mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2012;27:1173–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Tanaka H, Monahan KD, Seals DR. Age-predicted maximal heart rate revisited. J Am Coll Cardiol. 2001;37:153–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Gaskill SE, Ruby BC, Walker AJ, Sanchez OA, Serfass RC, Leon AS. Validity and reliability of combining three methods to determine ventilatory threshold. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2001;33:1841–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Levine TR, Hullett CR. Eta squared, partial eta squared, and misreporting of effect size in communication research. Hum Commun Res. 2002;28:612–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Ekkekakis P, Hall E, Petruzzello S. Practical markers of the transition from aerobic to anaerobic metabolism during exercise: rationale and a case for affect-based exercise prescription. Prev Med. 2004;38:149–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Fabre C, Massé-Biron J, Ahmaidi S, Adam B, Préfaut C. Effectiveness of individualized aerobic training at the ventilatory threshold in the elderly. J Gerontol Ser A Biol Med Sci. 1997;52:B260–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Parfitt G, Blisset A, Rose EA, Eston R. Physiological and perceptual responses to affect-regulated exercise in healthy young women. Psychophysiology. 2012;49:104–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Williams D, Dunsiger S, Jennings E, Marcus B. Does affective valence during and immediately following a 10-min walk predict concurrent and future physical activity? Ann Behav Med. 2012;44:43–51.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Perri MG, Anton SD, Durning PE, Ketterson TU, Sydeman SJ, Berlant NE, Kanasky WF Jr, Newton RL Jr, Limacher MC, Martin AD. Adherence to exercise prescriptions: effects of prescribing moderate versus higher levels of intensity and frequency. Health Psychol. 2002;21:452–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Hamlyn-Williams C, Freeman P, Parfitt G. Acute affective responses to prescribed and self-selected exercise sessions in adolescent girls: an observational study. BMC Sports Sci Med Rehab. 2014;6:35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Zenko Z, Ekkekakis P, Ariely D. Can you have your vigorous exercise and enjoy it too? Ramping intensity down increases postexercise, remembered, and forecasted pleasure. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2016;38(2):149–59.

  40. Gore CJ, Edwards DE. Australian fitness norms. The Health Development Foundation: Adelaide; 1992.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Eston RG, Parfitt G, Campbell L, Lamb KL. Reliability of effort perception for regulating exercise intensity in children using the cart and load effort rating (CALER) scale. Pediatr Exerc Sci. 2000;12:388–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Eston RG, Davies BL, Williams JG. Use of perceived effort ratings to control exercise intensity in young healthy adults. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol. 1987;56:222–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank and acknowledge the contribution of all participants involved.

The authors would also like to acknowledge that the findings from this work were presented as an oral presentation by LM at the 2016 9th World Congress of Active Ageing, Melbourne, AUSTRALIA.

Abstract reference: Smith AE, McAuliffe L, gray C, Eston RG and Parfitt GC (2016) ‘Can low active adults living with and without mild cognitive impairment regulate their exercise intensity with perceived exertion?’, V24, S12, journal of aging and physical activity.

Funding

This study was funded by an Alzheimer’s Australia Post-Doctoral Fellowship awarded to AS (DGP14–4). Expresso bikes were loaned to researchers from Michael Moroney for the duration of the study, fitness generation, Australia. Consumables for the study were funded by the School of Health Science and the School of Psychology, Social Work and Scoial policy, University of South Australia. AS is currently funded by an NHMRC-ARC Dementia Training Fellowship (APP1097397).

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

AS, LM, GP, RE and HK designed the study; AS, LM and CG collected and analysed the data; LM and CG drafted the manuscript; all authors edited and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ashleigh E. Smith.

Ethics declarations

Competing interest

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethics was approved by the University of South Australia Human Research Ethics Committee, protocol number 0000033994, approved on 19th March 2015. All participants were required to sign a consent form and participant data were de-identified in the results.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

McAuliffe, L., Parfitt, G.C., Eston, R.G. et al. Combining perceptual regulation and exergaming for exercise prescription in low-active adults with and without cognitive impairment. BMC Sports Sci Med Rehabil 10, 2 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13102-018-0091-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13102-018-0091-7

Keywords